
All iLive content is medically reviewed or fact checked to ensure as much factual accuracy as possible.
We have strict sourcing guidelines and only link to reputable media sites, academic research institutions and, whenever possible, medically peer reviewed studies. Note that the numbers in parentheses ([1], [2], etc.) are clickable links to these studies.
If you feel that any of our content is inaccurate, out-of-date, or otherwise questionable, please select it and press Ctrl + Enter.
'Low' Lead in Early Childhood Is Not Low Risk: How Even 1 µg/dL Affects Academic Performance
Last reviewed: 18.08.2025

Lead is a cumulative neurotoxin with no safe threshold for the developing brain. Even “trace” levels in early childhood are associated with poorer cognitive performance, behavioral problems, poor academic performance, and later socioeconomic losses. Yet current blood “reference” values (e.g., 3.5 mcg/dL) have long been used as a trigger for retesting and remediation, effectively dividing children into “below threshold” and “above threshold.”
What is already known
Sources of lead include old lead paint and dust in homes prior to the late 1970s, lead soldering and plumbing, contaminated roadside soil, some consumer products (unregulated ceramics/glazes, spices, cosmetics), and lead shot in game. The risk is unevenly distributed, with families living in older housing stock and areas with historical contamination being more severely affected. Epidemiological studies have shown for decades that IQ and academic performance declines occur at levels <10 and even <5 μg/dL.
In children with blood lead levels below 3.5 μg/dL as toddlers, each additional “unit” (+1 μg/dL) was associated with worse math and reading scores— almost as strongly as in children with “high” levels (≥3.5 μg/dL). The takeaway message is simple and unsettling: There is no safe threshold for lead in children’s brains, and current thresholds should be revised downward. The study is published in JAMA Network Open.
What the researchers did
- They linked birth certificates of all Iowa children (1989-2010), school test scores (grades 2-11), and early blood lead testing data.
- We received a data set of up to 305 thousand children and 1.78 million “child-class observations”.
- The average age at lead testing was 1.9 years (i.e. early exposure).
- 37.7% of children had lead <3.5 μg/dL (mean ~2.3), the rest ≥3.5 μg/dL (mean ~5.7).
- They estimated school national percentile ranks (NPR) in math and reading, taking into account a bunch of factors: gender, gestational age, birth weight, maternal age and education, smoking during pregnancy, school, year of testing, etc.
Key Results
- Among children with low levels (<3.5):
+1 μg/dL lead → -0.47 percentile point in math and -0.38 percentile point in reading. - Among children with high levels (≥3.5):
+1 μg/dL → -0.52 (math) and -0.56 (reading). - The reductions extend across all classes (2-11) rather than disappearing over time.
- Sensitive tests (excluding “fixed” values of 5 µg/dl in old laboratories, limiting by years, taking into account trends by region, etc.) do not change the picture.
"Half a percentile over 1 mcg/dL" seems like a small thing? At the level of one child, the effect is small. But at the level of an entire state/country with millions of children, that's thousands of "lost" high scores, fewer chances for advanced courses and admission, a larger gap in educational inequality. And most importantly, the effect lasts for years.
Why does this happen?
Lead is a neurotoxin. It interferes with synapse formation, myelination, neurotransmitter systems, and the fine-tuning of neural networks. Even trace amounts are important to the developing brain. That's why both the WHO and the CDC have long said there is no safe level of lead—it's just a question of the scale of the damage and the fairness of the interventions.
What does this mean for policy and practice?
- The threshold of 3.5 mcg/dl should be revised lower. Today it serves as a signal: who to recheck, where to look for sources of lead, who to help with diet/services. New data shows: "below the threshold" ≠ "safe for study".
- Shifting focus from high-level response to primary prevention:
- mass screening in early childhood (and repeated in risk regions);
- housing remediation (lead-based paint in pre-1978 homes, lead flashings/pipes, old windows and dust, contaminated roadside soil);
- water control (test kits, replacement of sections of the pipe “house-street”, flushing, if possible - filters);
- consumer source control: imported spices and cosmetics, uncertified ceramics/lead glazes, hunting ammunition (game);
- Nutrition: sufficient iron, calcium, vitamin C - reduces lead absorption.
- School support measures for children with identified impacts: early diagnosis and correction, tutoring in reading/mathematics - so that the “break in the curve” of academic performance does not become fixed.
Important Disclaimers
- This is an observational study: it shows an association, not "hard causation." But the results are consistent with dozens of previous studies—and biology is against lead.
- Iowa is mostly white; needs to be moved to more diverse states/cities.
- There was no data on family income/housing quality - residual mixedness is possible. However, the authors took into account many indirect indicators and made "strict" models by districts.
- In previous years, some laboratories rounded low values to 5 µg/dL - the authors checked this separately.
What should parents do?
- Do you live in a house built before 1978 or in an old housing development? Get lead tested: your child's blood (check with your pediatrician) and your home (paint/dust/soil/water).
- Try to keep dust under control: wet cleaning, HEPA vacuum cleaner, washing hands before eating, and keeping “outdoor” shoes outside.
- Kitchen and utensils: do not store acidic foods in unknown ceramics, be careful with imported spices/cosmetics.
- Nutrition: enough iron, calcium, vitamin C. Discuss with your doctor if your child is a picky eater.
- If lead is found, seek remediation through municipal programs; ask the school for early reading/math support.
Conclusion
Every 1 μg/dL of lead in the blood before kindergarten is a minus to the educational trajectory for many years, even if the value is below the "official" threshold. When it comes to the cognitive capital of a generation, there are no trifles. Politicians should lower thresholds and invest in prevention; pediatricians and schools should catch the risk with rank; families should know their sources and not be shy about demanding action.